Published by
Stanford Medicine

Category

Evolution

Behavioral Science, Evolution, Neuroscience, Research, Stanford News

We’ve got your number: Exact spot in brain where numeral recognition takes place revealed

weve-got-your-number-exact-spot-in-brain-where-numeral-recognition-takes-place-revealed

Your brain and my brain are shaped slightly differently. But, it’s a good bet, in almost the identical spot within each of them sits a clump of perhaps 1 to 2 million nerve cells that gets much more excited at the sight of numerals (“5,” for example) than when we see their spelled-out equivalents (“five”), lookalike letters (“5″ versus “S”) or scrambled symbols composed of rearranged components of the numerals themselves.

Josef Parvizi, MD, PhD, director of Stanford’s Human Intracranial Cognitive Electrophysiology Program, and his colleagues identified this numeral-recognition module by recording electrical activity directly from the brain surfaces of epileptic volunteers. Their study describing these experiments was just published in The Journal of Neuroscience.

As I explained in my release about the work:

[A]s a first step toward possible surgery to relieve unremitting seizures that weren’t responding to therapeutic drugs, [the patients had] had a small section of their skulls removed and electrodes applied directly to the brain’s surface. The procedure, which doesn’t destroy any brain tissue or disrupt the brain’s function, had been undertaken so that the patients could be monitored for several days to help attending neurologists find the exact location of their seizures’ origination points. While these patients are bedridden in the hospital for as much as a week of such monitoring, they are fully conscious, in no pain and, frankly, a bit bored.

Seven patients, in whom electrodes happened to be positioned near the area Parvizi’s team wanted to explore, gave the researchers permission to perform about an hour’s worth of tests. In the first, they watched a laptop screen on which appeared a rapid-fire random series of letters or numerals, scrambled versions of them, or foreign number symbols with which the experimental subjects were unfamiliar. In a second test, the experimental subjects viewed, again in thoroughly mixed-up sequence, numerals along with words for them as well as words that sounded the same (1″, “one”, “won”, “2″, “two”, “too”, etc.).

A region within a part of the brain called the inferior temporal gyrus showed activity in response to all kinds of squiggly lines, angles and curves. But within that area a small spot measuring about one-fifth of an inch across lit up preferentially in response to numerals compared with all the other stimuli.

The fact that this spot is embedded in a larger brain area generally responsive to lines, angles, and curves testifies to the human brain’s “plasticity:” its ability to tailor its form and function according to the dictates of experience.

“Humans aren’t born with the ability to recognize numbers,” says Parvizi. He thinks evolution may have generated, in the brains of our tree-dwelling primate ancestors, a brain region particularly adept at computing lines, angles and curves, facilitating snap decisions required for swinging quickly from one branch to the next.

Apparently, one particular spot within that larger tree-branch-interesection recognition area is easily diverted to the numeral-recognition activity constantly rewarded by parents and teachers during the numeracy boot camp called childhood.

Nobody can say those little monkeys don’t learn anything in kindergarten.

Previously: Metamorphosis: At the push of a button, a familiar face becomes a strange one and Why memory and math don’t mix: They require opposing states of the same brain circuitry
Photo by qthomasbower

Evolution, Immunology, Infectious Disease, Pediatrics, Research, Science, Stanford News

Deja Vu: Adults’ immune systems “remember” microscopic monsters they’ve never seen before

deja-vu-adults-immune-systems-remember-microscopic-monsters-theyve-never-seen-before

Probably no human whose age consists of two digits hasn’t at one time or another experienced a case of deja vu, the uncanny sense of having been through this (whatever “this” may be) before.

Well, it turns out that (as the scary narrator of a kitchy sci-fi TV series I inhaled with both nostrils as a kid might say about UFOs and the like), “We’re not alone…” Our own immune systems, among whose chief functions is to fight off invading pathogens, also entertain “memories” of infectous microbes they’ve never, ever encountered. And that’s a lucky thing.

A human has only 20,000 or so genes, so it’s tough to imagine just how our immune systems are able to recognize potentially billions of differently shaped microbial body parts (or “epitopes” in immune-speak). Stanford immunologist Mark Davis, PhD, tore the cover off of immunology in the early 1980s by solving that riddle.

Now, in a just published study in Immunity, Davis and his team have used an advanced technique developed in his lab in the 1990s to show that a surprising percentage of adult humans’ workhorse immune cells targeting one or another microbial epitope are poised to pounce on the particular epitope they target (and the bug it rode in with) despite having never come across it before. This hypervigilant configuration, called the ”memory” state, was previously supposed to be limited to immune cells that have previously had a run-in with the epitope of interest.

Davis think’s he’s got the dirt on what’s behind the phenomenon: Dirt. He reasons that the kind of immune cells in question have more flexibility than has been thought, so each of them can “cross-react” to a small set of similarly but not identically shaped ”lookalike” epitopes it’s never experienced. Our daily exposures to ubiquitous, mostly harmless micro-organisms that dwell in dirt, on doorknobs, and in our diets gradually produces an aggregate immune “memory” of not only these microbes’ body parts, but those of other bugs as well - including some nasty ones like HIV (the virus that causes AIDS), herpesvirus, and more.

Because cells in the “memory” configuration can react much, much faster to an infectious pathogen than “naive” cells targeting the exact same pathogen, this eerie foreknowledge can spell the difference between life and death.

But this immune memory still depends on having been exposed to something. In the study, the immune cells in blood from newborns’ umbilical cords showed no “memory” of anything at all.

As I wrote in my release on the new findings:

[This discovery] could explain why young children are so much more vulnerable to infectious diseases than adults. Moreover, the findings suggest a possible reason why vaccination against a single pathogen, measles, appears to have reduced overall mortality among African children more than can be attributed to the drop in measles deaths alone.

“It may even provide an evolutionary clue about why kids eat dirt,” Davis told me.

Previously: Immunology escapes from the mouse trap, Age-related drop in immune responsiveness may be reversible and Common genetic Alzheimer’s risk factor disrupts healthy older women’s brain function, but not men’s
Photo by Damian Gadal

Evolution, Genetics, Immunology, Infectious Disease, Pregnancy, Research, Stanford News

Revealed: Epic evolutionary struggle between reproduction and immunity to infectious disease

revealed-epic-evolutionary-struggle-between-reproduction-and-immunity-to-infectious-disease

Can’t blame us if our feet hurt. We humans have been walking erect for well over 3 million years.

That new style of locomotion necessitated “considerable anatomical changes that altered the size and shape of the human female pelvis and the dimensions of the birth canal,” write Stanford and evolutionary theorist Peter Parham, PhD, and pathologist Ashley Moffitt, MD, of the University of Cambridge, in a just-released review article in Nature Reviews Immunology.

Walking upright, along with the development of our bigger brains, allowed us to head out out of Africa into Eurasia. Successive migration events of this nature have occurred a number of times since - leading to the emergence of Neanderthals in Europe around 600,000 years ago and the arrival there of anatomically modern humans (that’s us) a scant 67,000 years ago, give or take a few weeks.

But those bigger brains caused problems, too, the authors write:

The size of the human baby’s head increased until it reached the limit defined by the birth canal… [A]t full term, a modern human baby’s head just fits into the birth canal… [I]n the course of human evolution, birthing became a difficult, dangerous and frequently fatal process…

Bigger brains need more nourishment in utero, putting greater demands on the blood supply to the placenta. Plus, insufficient blood supply to the placenta can lead to pre-eclampsia, stillbirth or low birth weight. But if an emerging baby’s head is too big, it could kill both mother and child on the way out of the womb. It’s a delicate balancing act.

To the rescue come specialized immune cells called natural killer (or NK) cells, which play an important role in our front-line defense against infectious pathogens. NK cells play a key role in reproduction, too, by carefully regulating the development of placental blood vessels. This keeps fetal growth in bounds.

NK cells feature a particular surface molecule that comes in two versions. One of these versions turns out to be somewhat better suited for the task of managing fetal growth, the other for fighting infectious pathogens. Both of these versions are found in every human population ever studied, suggesting that a group’s survival over evolutionary time is favored by some optimal balance between the two.

Parham and Moffett conjure up a vision of just how such a compromise between these two versions might arise:

When an epidemic infection passed through a population, causing disease, death (particularly of the young) and social disruption, selection favored [one version]. When the epidemic subsided, the surviving and now smaller population was immune to further infection and enriched for [that version]. At this juncture, survival of the current generation was no longer the issue, and the priority became production of the next generation. [This favored the evolutionary selection of] factors that enhance the generation of larger and more robust progeny… [H]uman history has always involved successive cycles of [this] type…

Thus, all human populations maintain a mix of both surface-molecule versions. Any distinction between safe, efficient reproduction and vulnerability to infectious disease may seem nonexistent to people who consider babies to be invading organisms - which I suppose they are, in a way. (But cute, too.)

Previously: Our species’ twisted family tree, Humans owe important disease-fighting genes to trysts with cavemen and Humans share history - and a fair amount of genetic material - with Neanderthals
Photo by Lord Jim

Scope will be on a limited publishing schedule in honor of Martin Luther King, Jr. Day. We’ll resume our normal schedule tomorrow.

Evolution, Genetics, Research, Stanford News

Dumb, dumber and dumbest? Stanford biologist suggests humans on a downward slide

dumb-dumber-and-dumbest-stanford-biologist-suggests-humans-on-a-downward-slide

I laughed out loud when I saw the many news reports today about the latest articles by Stanford developmental biologist Gerald Crabtree, MD. Not because the reports were wrong or because the research is poor. It’s just that the topic is guaranteed to make nearly anyone either laugh or cry — particularly someone (ahem, ME) who has recently been feeling less and less smart with each birthday.

Crabtree hypothesized in two articles published today in Trends in Genetics that humans are slowly accumulating genetic mutations that will have a deleterious effect on both our intellect and emotional stability. The reason, he believes, is the relative lack of selective pressure during the past 3,000 years. He begins boldly:

I would wager that if an average citizen from Athens of 1000 BC were to appear suddenly among us, he or she would be among the brightest and most intellectually alive of our colleagues and companions, with a good memory, a broad range of ideas, and a clear-sighted view of important issues. Furthermore, I would guess that he or she would be among the most emotionally stable of our friends and colleagues.

It’s an intriguing point. I’ve recently been re-educating myself about ancient Greek history,and I’ve been newly amazed about the breadth and depth of the philosophy and ideas propounded by people thousands of years ago.

Crabtree calculates that between 2,000 to 5,000 genes are likely required to maintain optimal intellectual acuity and emotional well-being. Extending his theory, it’s likely that we’ve each accumulated at least two harmful mutations during the intervening millennia. (Case in point? I just had to look up how to spell that last word.) Why? Well, according to Crabtree:

It is also likely that the need for intelligence was reduced as we began to live in supportive societies that made up for lapses of judgment or failures of comprehension. Community life would, I believe, tend to reduce the selective pressure placed on every individual, every day of their life. Indeed that is why I prefer to live in such a society.

Don’t we all? But although it’s disheartening to think that we’re locked in a downward spiral, it’s way too soon to panic. Crabtree emphasizes that our demise will be slow:

However, if such a study found accelerating rates of accumulation of deleterious alleles over the past several thousand years then we would have to think about these issues more seriously. But we would not have to think too fast. One does not need to imagine a day when we could no longer comprehend the problem, or counteract the slow decay in the genes underlying our intellectual fitness, or have visions of the world population docilely watching reruns on televisions they can no longer build. It is exceedingly unlikely that a few hundred years will make any difference for the rate of change that might be occurring.

Whew.

Photo by CollegeDegrees360

Evolution, Genetics, Research, Stanford News

How the cheetah gets its… stripes? Stanford geneticist cracks the code

how-the-cheetah-gets-its-stripes-stanford-geneticist-cracks-the-code

Cheetahs with stripes? Tabby cats with blotches? Researchers in the laboratory of Stanford geneticist Greg Barsh, MD, PhD, have pinpointed the cause of the unique coat patterns that give big and little cats their familiar periodic markings that allow even small children to distinguish a tiger from a cheetah. The research will be published tomorrow in Science. From our release:

The scientists found that the two felines share a biological mechanism responsible for both the elegant stripes on the tabby cat and the cheetah’s normally dappled coat. Dramatic changes to the normal patterns occur when this pathway is disrupted: The resulting house cat has swirled patches of color rather than orderly stripes, and the normally spotted cheetah sports thick, dark lines down its back.

“Mutation of a single gene causes stripes to become blotches, and spots to become stripes,” said [Barsh], emeritus professor of genetics and of pediatrics at Stanford and an investigator at the HudsonAlpha Institute.

The differences are so pronounced that biologists at first thought that cheetahs with the mutated gene belonged to an entirely different species. The rare animals became known as “king cheetahs,” while affected tabby cats received the less-regal moniker of “blotched.” (The more familiar, striped cat is known as a mackerel tabby.)

The Stanford researchers collaborated with colleagues at the National Cancer Institute and HudsonAlpha Institute for Biotechnology in Huntsville, Alabama to conduct the study on feral cats in Northern California and captive and wild cheetah populations in North America, South African and Namibia. The results have implications beyond natural history. Again from our release:

Barsh and his lab members have spent decades investigating how traditional laboratory animals such as mice develop specific coat colors. His previous work identified a variety of biologically important pathways that control more than just hair or skin color, and have been linked to brain degeneration, anemia and bone marrow failure. But laboratory mice don’t display the pattern variation seen in many mammals.

Photo by Greg Barsh

Evolution, Public Health, Research

Do placebos provide a mental cue to kickstart the immune system?

British scientists have uncovered some interesting insights into the body’s healing process that could help explain the placebo effect.

In a new study (subscription required), researchers developed a computer model to test a decade’s old theory (.pdf) originally put forth by former London School of Economics psychologist Nicholas Humphrey, PhD. Humphrey proposed that patients subconsciously respond to treatment, even if it’s a sugar pill, because of their strong belief in the medicine’s ability to fight the infection and aid in recovery without further depleting the body’s resources.

A New Scientist story published today describes the latest findings and how the new evidence supports Humphrey’s earlier paper:

It all starts with the observation that something similar to the placebo effect occurs in many animals, says Peter Trimmer, a biologist at the University of Bristol, UK. For instance, Siberian hamsters do little to fight an infection if the lights above their lab cage mimic the short days and long nights of winter. But changing the lighting pattern to give the impression of summer causes them to mount a full immune response.

Likewise, those people who think they are taking a drug but are really receiving a placebo can have a response which is twice that of those who receive no pills (Annals of Family Medicine, doi.org/cckm8b). In Siberian hamsters and people, intervention creates a mental cue that kick-starts the immune response.

Trimmer’s simulation is built on this assumption - that animals need to spend vital resources on fighting low-level infections. The model revealed that, in challenging environments, animals lived longer and sired more offspring if they endured infections without mounting an immune response. In more favourable environments, it was best for animals to mount an immune response and return to health as quickly as possible (Evolution and Human Behavior, doi.org/h8p). The results show a clear evolutionary benefit to switching the immune system on and off depending on environmental conditions.

Previously: Debating the placebo as treatment, The puzzling powers of the palacebo effect and The increasing power of the placebo
Photo by Lucy Reynell

Evolution, Infectious Disease, Neuroscience, Stanford News, Surgery

Stress hormones moonlight as immune-system traffic cops

stress-hormones-moonlight-as-immune-system-traffic-cops

Birds do it. Bees do it. Even fishies in the seas do it. They freak out when put into stress-arousing situations. A hormone-induced call-to-arms called the flight-or-fight response quickens the senses, douses pain sensation, and pumps mammalian muscles to full strength.

Short-term stress not only energizes muscles but mobilizes immune cells, a just-published paper in the journal Psychoneuroendocrinology, spearheaded by Stanford’s Firdaus Dhabhar, PhD, shows. And that’s a good thing.

I know, I know. As I wrote in my press release on this development:

You’ve heard it a thousand times: Stress is bad for you. And it’s certainly true that chronic stress, lasting weeks and months, has deleterious effects including, notably, suppression of the immune response.

But short-term stress, a mobilization of bodily resources lasting minutes or hours in response to immediate threats, stimulates immune activity.

Dhabhar gently confined rats inside amply ventilated Plexiglas enclosures, inducing mild stress. (Think: “office cubies.”) Then he meticulously plotted the paths of several key immune-cell types. These cells responded like combat troops to squirts of three major stress hormones released from the rats’ adrenal glands on orders from the brain. First, they spilled out of their rest-and-recuperation reservoirs, the spleen and bone marrow, to the boulevard that is the bloodstream. Then - depending on the identities, timing and amounts of the stress hormones secreted - different immune-cell subsets parachuted out of circulation into potential front-line battle zones such as the skin and mucous membranes.

Which makes evolutionary sense, says Dhabhar. The immune system is crucial for wound healing and preventing or fending off infections, common risks during chases, escapes, and combat. “Mother Nature gave us the fight-or-flight site stress response to help us, not to kill us,” he says.

Dhabhar’s study caps over a decade of research into how the the brain manipulates the immune system with the aid of stress hormones. A few years ago he found that knee-surgery patients recovered faster and better if, during surgery, their immune-cell populations redistributed similarly to the pattern seen in the healthy rats in this new study. If those cells merely flatlined in response to the inevitable stress-hormone storm released during surgery, outcomes were poorer. (The latter pattern resembles what happens under conditions of chronic stress: a new bout of stress-hormone secretion just evokes a collective yawn from the body’s immune-cell army, which has been on alert so long it’s starting to sleep on the job.)

Imagine if this mechanism evolution built into us to help us get out of jams in the wild were domesticated. Dhabhar foresees a day when patients’ stress-hormone and immune status could be monitored before surgery or just prior to vaccination, and then precise, precisely timed doses of specific stress hormones could be given to optimize immune responsiveness. Conversely, crucial immune cells could be hormonally directed to hole up in safe havens such as the spleen while a cancer patient was getting chemotherapy or radiation, or to quiet down in a patient with an autoimmune disorder.

(The more quotidian takeaway: Life’s little stresses benefit your health. Just make sure to exhale.)

Photo by spaceamoeba

Evolution, Genetics, Research, Stanford News

Blond hair evolved more than once - and why it matters

blond-hair-evolved-more-than-once-and-why-it-matters

In a story online today, I describe how a team led by Stanford’s Carlos Bustamante, PhD, and Sean Myles, PhD, discovered that the gene for blond hair arose independently in the South Pacific nation of the Solomon Islands. The researchers identified the islanders’ “blond” gene and discovered it’s not the same gene that causes blond hair in Europeans.

Reporting the story was so much fun. I learned from Myles, who spent a month there gathering the data, how important it is to get the village chief on board when doing genetic research in the Solomon Islands, that Solomon Islanders’ saliva (which he collected for the study) is often bright red from chewing betel nut, and that on one of the islands (with an active volcano) you can cook by simply burying the food in the hot ground for a while. “The beaches are pristine, water super blue. You can hear the fish jumping in the water. It’s what a lot of people would think of as paradise,” he told me. Wow, what a trip.

The genetic finding, published in the May 4 issue of Science, has a certain “wow” appeal too, and the researchers say there’s a serious message that comes along with it. As Bustamante told me, the finding underscores the importance of genetic studies on isolated populations like the Solomon Islanders:

If we’re going to be designing the next generation of medical treatments using genetic information and we don’t have a really broad spectrum of populations included, you could disproportionately benefit some populations and harm others.

Myles, now an assistant professor at Nova Scotia Agricultural College, expands on this on his lab’s website:

Our result is therefore a call for action. We must take steps now to ensure that the benefits of current genomics research extend beyond privileged populations and provide an increase in well-being for people everywhere. Humanity’s natural genetic diversity is vast and fascinating – we should be measuring and assessing it all! The same applies for genomics research in agriculture. A continued focus on a small number of elite individuals in plant and animal breeding is myopic and dangerous. An immense amount of existing genetic diversity that is essential to our future well-being is being ignored. Whether it’s us or our food that we research, our aim is to cultivate an appreciation for natural genetic diversity. Our future depends on it.

Photo of a child from the Solomon Islands by Sean Myles

Evolution, Infectious Disease, Microbiology, Research, Stanford News

Shrugging off bugs: there’s more to beating infections than just fighting them

shrugging-off-bugs-theres-more-to-beating-infections-than-just-fighting-them

David Schneider, PhD, has used used two kinds of bugs (fruit flies and bacteria) to great effect, teasing out intriguing insights into the effects of sleep and caloric intake on response to infection.

Much has been written about the power of the immune system to stave off infectious disease; not so much about the damage the immune response itself can inflict on the organism it’s designed to stave. If you’ve ever ever had influenza, you’re well aware of how rotten it made you feel. That was largely a byproduct of your immune response, which, in its zeal to destroy microbial pathogens, often attacks not only infected but healthy tissue. It also raises your body temperature to uncomfortable heights and squirts out inflammatory molecules and oxidants that, while nipping the bug in the bud, may weaken innocent nearby cells and render them more vulnerable to maladies later on.

A delicate balance must be achieved between destroying the invader and sparing the host. To the rescue comes another, less-talked-about aspect of disease control Schneider calls “tolerance” - not to be confused with “immunological tolerance,” the willingness of the immune system not to respond with a vengeance to every damned molecule it meets along the way.

For example, an animal may amp up production of anti-oxidant molecules to scavenge free radicals that are released by immune activity in response to an infection. Or, beset by the parasite that causes malaria, it might boost its output of a special molecule that mops up hemoglobin, which, when released from ruptured red blood cells into the circulation, can severely damage tissues.

In a just-out review article in Science, Schneider and two co-authors emphasize the importance of tolerance, a collection of mechanisms that increase an animal’s ability to withstand infection independently of its ability to clear the pathogen itself. They write:

The concept of tolerance may… be applicable to the “Typhoid Mary” phenomenon. Healthy carriers that remain asymptomatic despite being infected are likely to have a high level of tolerance to the pathogen with which they are infected.

As above, so below. In the world of one-celled invaders and, unfortunately, in the larger world we inhabit, we sometimes have to fight back even at the cost of considerable damage to the home front. And, in both these worlds, it seems, victory results from a mix of fighting and enduring.

Previously: Fruit flies fly while scalped, exposing their brain cells for science

Evolution, Genetics, In the News, Infectious Disease, Science

Our species’ twisted family tree

our-species-twisted-family-tree

In second grade I got influenza and was bedridden for a week. On the advice of Mrs. Pils, our school librarian, my mom brought home a book for me to read: “Turi of the Magic Fingers,” a coming-of-age-cavekid tale that left me forever curious about our evolutionary past.

It turns out that the chain of matings that led up to us is multi-branched, as this story in today’s New York Times explains. Rather than a smooth chronological descent from a point-source somewhere in Africa, our DNA has been cobbled together from not only northward-bound Cro Magnon migrants but also at least two sets of folks they encountered, and apparently vanquished, on the way: Neanderthals in Europe, and Denisovans in Siberia.

And a lucky thing, too. Work by Stanford immunologist Peter Parham, PhD, reprised in the Times article, suggests that immunologically relevant snippets of DNA of Neanderthal and Denisovan origin that we mongrelized moderns retain to this day have proven instrumental in protecting us from diseases to which Cro Magnons hadn’t been previously exposed.

From the Times piece:

The value of the interbreeding shows up in the immune system, Dr. Parham’s analysis suggests. The Neanderthals and Denisovans had lived in Europe and Asia for many thousands of years before modern humans showed up and had developed ways to fight the diseases there… When modern humans mated with them, they got an injection of helpful genetic immune material, so useful that it remains in the genome today. This suggests that modern humans needed the archaic DNA to survive.

Nobody’s saying it was love - no doubt more of a club-carrying-caveboy-meets-cowering-conquered-cavegirl thing - but the resulting hybrid vigor has perhaps been the saving grace of today’s human race.

Previously: Humans owe important disease-fighting genes to trysts with cavemen and Humans share history – and a fair amount of genetic material – with Neanderthals
Photo by kevin dooley

Stanford Medicine Resources: